Popular version of paper 4pSC8 Taken together, these findings confirm that babies are highly attentive to
the acoustical environment that surrounds them. With respect to music, researchers
have not explored infant perception of music that is considered complicated
by musical standards. This bias falls in line with the advice of music educators
who often recommend simple music for the infant ear. Our finding that infants
can remember a piece of complex music over a 2-week delay appears to challenge
this implicit assumption that infants are ill-equipped to handle complex music.
To investigate whether babies can remember complex music, we exposed a
group of 8-month-old infants to one of two piano pieces, Forlane or Prelude,
from the composition "Le Tombeau de Couperin" by Maurice Ravel (1875-1937).
Musicians agree that this work is complicated due to its intricate harmonies,
complex rhythmic motives and textures (see excerpts below). The parents of each
child were given a CD recording of either the Forlane or Prelude movement and
were asked to play the assigned piece three times daily for the baby for ten
consecutive days. Parents also completed a diary to document listening date,
time and infant mood. Diaries and CDs were collected at the end of the prescribed
listening period. Babies were brought to the university lab for testing two
weeks following the CD collection date.
In
the lab, we tested each baby to see if they had a listening preference for one
of the two piano pieces using the Headturn Preference Procedure3 (see illustration
below).
The Headturn
Preference Procedure Our results showed that these infants displayed a preference for the piano
piece they heard. Babies exposed to the Forlane preferred to listen to it over
the Prelude and likewise, babies exposed to the Prelude piece preferred it over
the Forlane. We also tested a second group of babies who had never heard either
piece of music. These babies showed no preferences for either piece of music.
Taken together, these results show that babies had formed an impression of the
piano piece and were able to retain this impression over a 2-week delay.
To better understand whether complicated music is harder for babies to remember
than simple music, we compared our results with those found in a previous study
that used the same design and infants of the same age to see if infants could
remember a simple Mozart piece. This comparison shows that the preference effect
(the difference in listening time to the familiar versus the unfamiliar piece)
is stronger for more simple music. Thus, simple music may still be easier for
babies to encode than complicated music. Yet, babies were able to learn with
both simple and complex music. We don't know what information babies retained
from their exposure to either simple or complex music; was it the melody, or
some more global acoustic features of the music? This will require further systematic
investigation.
In sum, our study provides further proof that babies are keen listeners, able
to pick up and store information that is repeatedly presented in the acoustical
environment that surrounds them. Evidence that infants can remember complex
music is important for at least two reasons. Firstly, it helps us understand
whether babies have limitations to process music; if there is in fact music
that is appropriate for babies. Secondly, it is helpful for the elaboration
of curriculum for early childhood education. There are many related questions
to answer to fully address these general issues. What are the long-term effects
of such learning? Do children remember the music they heard when they were babies?
To what extent do musical experiences in infancy dictate people's later musical
taste? While we attempt to answer these questions, keep in mind that your baby
is also listening to that piano CD that you love so much. And learning. And
remembering.
Main
references:
1Jusczyk, P.W. & Hohne, E. A. (1997). Infants' memory for spoken
words. Science, 277, 1984-1986. 3 Kemler-Nelson, D.G., Jusczyk, P.W., Mandel, D.R., Myers, J., Turk,
A., & Gerken, L. (1995). The head-turn preference procedure for testing
auditory perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 111-116.
This paper will presented at the 143rd Meeting of the Acoustical
Society of America on Thursday, June 6th at 1PM during session
4pSC - Speech Communication and Psychological and Physiological Acoustics:
Future of Infant Speech Perception Research: Session in Memory of Peter Jusczyk.
For
further information, please contact : beatriz.ilari@mail.mcgill.ca
Beatriz Ilari1, Linda Polka2 & Eugenia Costa-Giomi1
-beatriz.ilari@mail.mcgill.ca
McGill University, Montreal,
Canada
Faculty of Music1, School of Communication Sciences and Disorders2
Presented Thursday afternoon, June 6, 2002
143rd ASA Meeting, Pittsburgh, PA
Babies are great listeners. Hours from birth, a newborn can tell his mother's
voice from that of another woman. By about 4 months of age, a baby smiles and
shows recognition of his own name. Babies between 8 and 9 months of age can
remember words from a story1 or a simple piece of music2
that they heard previously, even after a two-week delay.
2 Saffran, J.R., Loman, M.M., Robertson, R.R.W. (2000). Infant
memory for musical experiences. Cognition,
77, B15-B23.