[ Lay Language Paper Index | Press Room ]
Is Clear Speech Equally Clear for All Listeners?
Ann Bradlow - abradlow@northwestern.edu
Department of Linguistics
Northwestern University
2016 Sheridan Road
Evanston, IL 60208
Popular version of paper 4aSC1
Presented Thursday Morning, May 1, 2003
145th ASA Meeting, Nashville, TN
Imagine that you are travelling in a country where you have only a passing knowledge of the language. You slip on a special speech enhancement device and suddenly the foreign-language speech around you seems so much easier to understand. The device that you slipped on works by picking up the speech signal of a person you are talking to, enhancing the signal, and then delivering it to your ears through headphones. This is not a simple amplification device or a translation machine, rather it enhances the speech signal such that you can now more easily decipher the language of your current environment. For example, it makes word boundaries more salient and clarifies the pronunciation of all of the individual speech sounds.
Would the same device work for all languages or would you need a language-specific
device? And, would the same device work for listeners with hearing impairments
and for listeners in particularly noisy environments? By comparing the
responses of different listener groups to naturally produced clear speech,
our work has shown that the language experience of the listener plays an
important role in determining speech intelligibility, suggesting that the
optimal speech enhancement device will have to be customized to the needs
of the listener and listening situation.
What is naturally produced clear speech?
When a talker believes that the listener is likely to have speech perception
difficulties due to a hearing loss, the presence of some background noise,
or a different native language background, she or he will typically adopt
a "clear" speaking style that is distinct from her or his typical conversational
speaking style. From careful acoustic comparisons of English conversational
and clear speech samples, we know that English clear speech is typically
slower and more precisely articulated than English conversational speech.
Furthermore, these acoustic studies have demonstrated that the English
clear speech enhancements reflect specific features of the sound structure
of the language. For example, in English some vowels are always longer
than others (e.g. the vowels in the words "feet, boot, and late" are always
longer than the vowels in "fit, put, and let".) In clear speech, all vowels
are lengthened (because of the slower rate of speech) but long vowels are
lengthened more than short vowels. In other words, clear speech enhances
the length contrast between English long and short vowels rather
than just lengthening all vowels by the same amount.
Who benefits from clear speech?
Previous research has established that adult English listeners with
either impaired or normal hearing typically find English clear speech more
intelligible than English conversational speech. We wondered whether listeners
with less advanced skills in the target language (in this case, American
English) would get as great a speech perception benefit from clear speech
as listeners with more advanced target language skills such as the adults
tested in the earlier studies. We reasoned that since less advanced listeners
(e.g. non-native listeners) may not be well practiced in decoding English
speech signals, they may not know how to take advantage of all of the clear
speech enhancements that talkers put into effect when producing clear speech.
For example, if a listener is not already sensitive to the vowel length
difference described above in many languages vowels do not differ in
terms of length as they do in English - then the enhanced vowel length
contrast of English clear speech will not be particularly helpful.
We hoped that finding out exactly who benefits from naturally produced clear speech would help delineate the features of clear speech that are most responsible for its enhanced intelligibility for specific listener populations.
A test of the clear speech benefit across populations.
In our work, we have been testing the conversational and clear speech
perception abilities of various listener populations who vary in their
language development. We tested children (8-12 years old), adults who are
native speakers of English and adults who are non-native speakers of English.
(The children were also native speakers of English). Heres how the
test works: A participant listens to sentences over headphones and then
simply writes down what she or he hears. The sentences are all simple English
sentences produced by a talker in either conversational or clear speech.
To make the task challenging, the sentence recordings are mixed with noise;
this also reflects real-world listening conditions better than perfectly
"clean" recordings. When we do this test with kids, instead of writing
down what they hear, they repeat the sentence to the experimenter.
Click here to hear examples of the stimuli we use in the clear speech test.
As you can see in the figure below, our results show that the size of
the clear speech benefit varies across listener groups: the native listener
adults showed a large clear speech benefit while the kids and non-native
listeners showed smaller benefits. We interpret this as reflecting a difference
in language development: the adult native listeners were best able to take
advantage of the clear speech enhancements because they have had the most
experience with spoken English and are therefore most sensitive to all
of the subtle enhancement cues that the talkers put into the clear speech
productions.
Conclusions and future directions.
Based on this cross-population comparison of the clear speech benefit
we are beginning to see how clear speech intelligibility depends not only
on talker characteristics (previous research has shown that some
talkers are better clear speech producers than others) but also on listener
characteristics. Talkers can enhance the intelligibility of their speech
by going into a clear speech mode, but the effectiveness of the enhancements
depends on the experience of the listener. By characterizing both the talker
and listener factors that contribute to speech intelligibility we may eventually
be able to develop automated speech intelligibility enhancement strategies
that will be optimized for a wide range of speech communication situations.
In parallel work, we are also investigating clear speech characteristics in other languages. This should help us delineate which clear speech features are universal and which are language specific, thereby bringing us closer to the ultimate goal of applying these findings to the development of automated speech enhancement devices that could convert a conversational speech signal into a highly intelligible, clear speech signal in a wide range of languages as well as a wide range of listening situations.